Merge from emacs-24; up to 2012-05-08T14:11:47Z!monnier@iro.umontreal.ca
[bpt/emacs.git] / etc / MOTIVATION
CommitLineData
1bac2ebb
DL
1STUDIES FIND REWARD OFTEN NO MOTIVATOR
2
3Creativity and intrinsic interest diminish if task is done for gain
4
5By Alfie Kohn
6Special to the Boston Globe
7[reprinted with permission of the author
8 from the Monday 19 January 1987 Boston Globe]
9
b727702b
EZ
10Verbatim copying and distribution is permitted in any medium
11provided this notice is preserved.
12
1bac2ebb
DL
13In the laboratory, rats get Rice Krispies. In the classroom the top
14students get A's, and in the factory or office the best workers get
15raises. It's an article of faith for most of us that rewards promote
16better performance.
17
18But a growing body of research suggests that this law is not nearly as
19ironclad as was once thought. Psychologists have been finding that
20rewards can lower performance levels, especially when the performance
21involves creativity.
22
23A related series of studies shows that intrinsic interest in a task -
24the sense that something is worth doing for its own sake - typically
25declines when someone is rewarded for doing it.
26
27If a reward - money, awards, praise, or winning a contest - comes to
28be seen as the reason one is engaging in an activity, that activity
29will be viewed as less enjoyable in its own right.
30
31With the exception of some behaviorists who doubt the very existence
32of intrinsic motivation, these conclusions are now widely accepted
33among psychologists. Taken together, they suggest we may unwittingly
34be squelching interest and discouraging innovation among workers,
35students and artists.
36
37The recognition that rewards can have counter-productive effects is
38based on a variety of studies, which have come up with such findings
39as these: Young children who are rewarded for drawing are less likely
40to draw on their own that are children who draw just for the fun of
41it. Teenagers offered rewards for playing word games enjoy the games
42less and do not do as well as those who play with no rewards.
43Employees who are praised for meeting a manager's expectations suffer
44a drop in motivation.
45
46Much of the research on creativity and motivation has been performed
47by Theresa Amabile, associate professor of psychology at Brandeis
48University. In a paper published early last year on her most recent
49study, she reported on experiments involving elementary school and
50college students. Both groups were asked to make "silly" collages.
51The young children were also asked to invent stories.
52
53The least-creative projects, as rated by several teachers, were done
54by those students who had contracted for rewards. "It may be that
55commissioned work will, in general, be less creative than work that is
56done out of pure interest," Amabile said.
57
58In 1985, Amabile asked 72 creative writers at Brandeis and at Boston
59University to write poetry. Some students then were given a list of
60extrinsic (external) reasons for writing, such as impressing teachers,
61making money and getting into graduate school, and were asked to think
62about their own writing with respect to these reasons. Others were
63given a list of intrinsic reasons: the enjoyment of playing with
64words, satisfaction from self-expression, and so forth. A third group
65was not given any list. All were then asked to do more writing.
66
67The results were clear. Students given the extrinsic reasons not only
68wrote less creatively than the others, as judged by 12 independent
69poets, but the quality of their work dropped significantly. Rewards,
70Amabile says, have this destructive effect primarily with creative
71tasks, including higher-level problem-solving. "The more complex the
72activity, the more it's hurt by extrinsic reward," she said.
73
74But other research shows that artists are by no means the only ones
75affected.
76
77In one study, girls in the fifth and sixth grades tutored younger
78children much less effectively if they were promised free movie
79tickets for teaching well. The study, by James Gabarino, now
80president of Chicago's Erikson Institute for Advanced Studies in Child
81Development, showed that tutors working for the reward took longer to
82communicate ideas, got frustrated more easily, and did a poorer job in
83the end than those who were not rewarded.
84
85Such findings call into question the widespread belief that money is
86an effective and even necessary way to motivate people. They also
87challenge the behaviorist assumption that any activity is more likely
88to occur if it is rewarded. Amabile says her research "definitely
89refutes the notion that creativity can be operantly conditioned."
90
91But Kenneth McGraw, associate professor of psychology at the
92University of Mississippi, cautions that this does not mean
93behaviorism itself has been invalidated. "The basic principles of
94reinforcement and rewards certainly work, but in a restricted context"
95- restricted, that is, to tasks that are not especially interesting.
96
97Researchers offer several explanations for their surprising findings
98about rewards and performance.
99
100First, rewards encourage people to focus narrowly on a task, to do it
101as quickly as possible and to take few risks. "If they feel that
00f9fb8b 102'this is something I have to get through to get the prize,' they're
1bac2ebb
DL
103going to be less creative," Amabile said.
104
105Second, people come to see themselves as being controlled by the
106reward. They feel less autonomous, and this may interfere with
107performance. "To the extent one's experience of being
108self-determined is limited," said Richard Ryan, associate psychology
109professor at the University of Rochester, "one's creativity will be
110reduced as well."
111
112Finally, extrinsic rewards can erode intrinsic interest. People who
113see themselves as working for money, approval or competitive success
114find their tasks less pleasurable, and therefore do not do them as
115well.
116
117The last explanation reflects 15 years of work by Ryan's mentor at the
118University of Rochester, Edward Deci. In 1971, Deci showed that
119"money may work to buy off one's intrinsic motivation for an activity"
120on a long-term basis. Ten years later, Deci and his colleagues
121demonstrated that trying to best others has the same effect. Students
122who competed to solve a puzzle quickly were less likely than those who
123were not competing to keep working at it once the experiment was over.
124
125Control plays role
126
127There is general agreement, however, that not all rewards have the
128same effect. Offering a flat fee for participating in an experiment -
129similar to an hourly wage in the workplace - usually does not reduce
130intrinsic motivation. It is only when the rewards are based on
131performing a given task or doing a good job at it - analogous to
132piece-rate payment and bonuses, respectively - that the problem
133develops.
134
135The key, then, lies in how a reward is experienced. If we come to
136view ourselves as working to get something, we will no longer find
137that activity worth doing in its own right.
138
139There is an old joke that nicely illustrates the principle. An
140elderly man, harassed by the taunts of neighborhood children, finally
141devises a scheme. He offered to pay each child a dollar if they would
142all return Tuesday and yell their insults again. They did so eagerly
143and received the money, but he told them he could only pay 25 cents on
144Wednesday. When they returned, insulted him again and collected their
145quarters, he informed them that Thursday's rate would be just a penny.
146"Forget it," they said - and never taunted him again.
147
148Means to and end
149
150In a 1982 study, Stanford psychologist Mark L. Lepper showed that any
151task, no matter how enjoyable it once seemed, would be devalued if it
152were presented as a means rather than an end. He told a group of
153preschoolers they could not engage in one activity they liked until
154they first took part in another. Although they had enjoyed both
155activities equally, the children came to dislike the task that was a
156prerequisite for the other.
157
158It should not be surprising that when verbal feedback is experienced
159as controlling, the effect on motivation can be similar to that of
160payment. In a study of corporate employees, Ryan found that those who
161were told, "Good, you're doing as you /should/" were "significantly
162less intrinsically motivated than those who received feedback
163informationally."
164
165There's a difference, Ryan says, between saying, "I'm giving you this
166reward because I recognize the value of your work" and "You're getting
167this reward because you've lived up to my standards."
168
169A different but related set of problems exists in the case of
170creativity. Artists must make a living, of course, but Amabile
171emphasizes that "the negative impact on creativity of working for
172rewards can be minimized" by playing down the significance of these
173rewards and trying not to use them in a controlling way. Creative
174work, the research suggests, cannot be forced, but only allowed to
175happen.
176
177/Alfie Kohn, a Cambridge, MA writer, is the author of "No Contest: The
178Case Against Competition," recently published by Houghton Mifflin Co.,
179Boston, MA. ISBN 0-395-39387-6. /