(update_text_area): Set phys_cursor_on_p to 0 in the
[bpt/emacs.git] / etc / MOTIVATION
CommitLineData
1bac2ebb
DL
1STUDIES FIND REWARD OFTEN NO MOTIVATOR
2
3Creativity and intrinsic interest diminish if task is done for gain
4
5By Alfie Kohn
6Special to the Boston Globe
7[reprinted with permission of the author
8 from the Monday 19 January 1987 Boston Globe]
9
10In the laboratory, rats get Rice Krispies. In the classroom the top
11students get A's, and in the factory or office the best workers get
12raises. It's an article of faith for most of us that rewards promote
13better performance.
14
15But a growing body of research suggests that this law is not nearly as
16ironclad as was once thought. Psychologists have been finding that
17rewards can lower performance levels, especially when the performance
18involves creativity.
19
20A related series of studies shows that intrinsic interest in a task -
21the sense that something is worth doing for its own sake - typically
22declines when someone is rewarded for doing it.
23
24If a reward - money, awards, praise, or winning a contest - comes to
25be seen as the reason one is engaging in an activity, that activity
26will be viewed as less enjoyable in its own right.
27
28With the exception of some behaviorists who doubt the very existence
29of intrinsic motivation, these conclusions are now widely accepted
30among psychologists. Taken together, they suggest we may unwittingly
31be squelching interest and discouraging innovation among workers,
32students and artists.
33
34The recognition that rewards can have counter-productive effects is
35based on a variety of studies, which have come up with such findings
36as these: Young children who are rewarded for drawing are less likely
37to draw on their own that are children who draw just for the fun of
38it. Teenagers offered rewards for playing word games enjoy the games
39less and do not do as well as those who play with no rewards.
40Employees who are praised for meeting a manager's expectations suffer
41a drop in motivation.
42
43Much of the research on creativity and motivation has been performed
44by Theresa Amabile, associate professor of psychology at Brandeis
45University. In a paper published early last year on her most recent
46study, she reported on experiments involving elementary school and
47college students. Both groups were asked to make "silly" collages.
48The young children were also asked to invent stories.
49
50The least-creative projects, as rated by several teachers, were done
51by those students who had contracted for rewards. "It may be that
52commissioned work will, in general, be less creative than work that is
53done out of pure interest," Amabile said.
54
55In 1985, Amabile asked 72 creative writers at Brandeis and at Boston
56University to write poetry. Some students then were given a list of
57extrinsic (external) reasons for writing, such as impressing teachers,
58making money and getting into graduate school, and were asked to think
59about their own writing with respect to these reasons. Others were
60given a list of intrinsic reasons: the enjoyment of playing with
61words, satisfaction from self-expression, and so forth. A third group
62was not given any list. All were then asked to do more writing.
63
64The results were clear. Students given the extrinsic reasons not only
65wrote less creatively than the others, as judged by 12 independent
66poets, but the quality of their work dropped significantly. Rewards,
67Amabile says, have this destructive effect primarily with creative
68tasks, including higher-level problem-solving. "The more complex the
69activity, the more it's hurt by extrinsic reward," she said.
70
71But other research shows that artists are by no means the only ones
72affected.
73
74In one study, girls in the fifth and sixth grades tutored younger
75children much less effectively if they were promised free movie
76tickets for teaching well. The study, by James Gabarino, now
77president of Chicago's Erikson Institute for Advanced Studies in Child
78Development, showed that tutors working for the reward took longer to
79communicate ideas, got frustrated more easily, and did a poorer job in
80the end than those who were not rewarded.
81
82Such findings call into question the widespread belief that money is
83an effective and even necessary way to motivate people. They also
84challenge the behaviorist assumption that any activity is more likely
85to occur if it is rewarded. Amabile says her research "definitely
86refutes the notion that creativity can be operantly conditioned."
87
88But Kenneth McGraw, associate professor of psychology at the
89University of Mississippi, cautions that this does not mean
90behaviorism itself has been invalidated. "The basic principles of
91reinforcement and rewards certainly work, but in a restricted context"
92- restricted, that is, to tasks that are not especially interesting.
93
94Researchers offer several explanations for their surprising findings
95about rewards and performance.
96
97First, rewards encourage people to focus narrowly on a task, to do it
98as quickly as possible and to take few risks. "If they feel that
99'this is something I hve to get through to get the prize,' the're
100going to be less creative," Amabile said.
101
102Second, people come to see themselves as being controlled by the
103reward. They feel less autonomous, and this may interfere with
104performance. "To the extent one's experience of being
105self-determined is limited," said Richard Ryan, associate psychology
106professor at the University of Rochester, "one's creativity will be
107reduced as well."
108
109Finally, extrinsic rewards can erode intrinsic interest. People who
110see themselves as working for money, approval or competitive success
111find their tasks less pleasurable, and therefore do not do them as
112well.
113
114The last explanation reflects 15 years of work by Ryan's mentor at the
115University of Rochester, Edward Deci. In 1971, Deci showed that
116"money may work to buy off one's intrinsic motivation for an activity"
117on a long-term basis. Ten years later, Deci and his colleagues
118demonstrated that trying to best others has the same effect. Students
119who competed to solve a puzzle quickly were less likely than those who
120were not competing to keep working at it once the experiment was over.
121
122Control plays role
123
124There is general agreement, however, that not all rewards have the
125same effect. Offering a flat fee for participating in an experiment -
126similar to an hourly wage in the workplace - usually does not reduce
127intrinsic motivation. It is only when the rewards are based on
128performing a given task or doing a good job at it - analogous to
129piece-rate payment and bonuses, respectively - that the problem
130develops.
131
132The key, then, lies in how a reward is experienced. If we come to
133view ourselves as working to get something, we will no longer find
134that activity worth doing in its own right.
135
136There is an old joke that nicely illustrates the principle. An
137elderly man, harassed by the taunts of neighborhood children, finally
138devises a scheme. He offered to pay each child a dollar if they would
139all return Tuesday and yell their insults again. They did so eagerly
140and received the money, but he told them he could only pay 25 cents on
141Wednesday. When they returned, insulted him again and collected their
142quarters, he informed them that Thursday's rate would be just a penny.
143"Forget it," they said - and never taunted him again.
144
145Means to and end
146
147In a 1982 study, Stanford psychologist Mark L. Lepper showed that any
148task, no matter how enjoyable it once seemed, would be devalued if it
149were presented as a means rather than an end. He told a group of
150preschoolers they could not engage in one activity they liked until
151they first took part in another. Although they had enjoyed both
152activities equally, the children came to dislike the task that was a
153prerequisite for the other.
154
155It should not be surprising that when verbal feedback is experienced
156as controlling, the effect on motivation can be similar to that of
157payment. In a study of corporate employees, Ryan found that those who
158were told, "Good, you're doing as you /should/" were "significantly
159less intrinsically motivated than those who received feedback
160informationally."
161
162There's a difference, Ryan says, between saying, "I'm giving you this
163reward because I recognize the value of your work" and "You're getting
164this reward because you've lived up to my standards."
165
166A different but related set of problems exists in the case of
167creativity. Artists must make a living, of course, but Amabile
168emphasizes that "the negative impact on creativity of working for
169rewards can be minimized" by playing down the significance of these
170rewards and trying not to use them in a controlling way. Creative
171work, the research suggests, cannot be forced, but only allowed to
172happen.
173
174/Alfie Kohn, a Cambridge, MA writer, is the author of "No Contest: The
175Case Against Competition," recently published by Houghton Mifflin Co.,
176Boston, MA. ISBN 0-395-39387-6. /